Emotion suppression and mortality risk over a 12-year follow-up

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychores.2013.07.014Get rights and content

Abstract

Objective

Suppression of emotion has long been suspected to have a role in health, but empirical work has yielded mixed findings. We examined the association between emotion suppression and all-cause, cardiovascular, and cancer mortality over 12 years of follow-up in a nationally representative US sample.

Methods

We used the 2008 General Social Survey–National Death Index (GSS–NDI) cohort, which included an emotion suppression scale administered to 729 people in 1996. Prospective mortality follow up between 1996 and 2008 of 111 deaths (37 by cardiovascular disease, 34 by cancer) was evaluated using Cox proportional hazards models adjusted for age, gender, education, and minority race/ethnicity.

Results

The 75th vs. 25th percentile on the emotional suppression score was associated with hazard ratio (HR) of 1.35 (95% Confidence Interval [95% CI] = 1.00, 1.82; P = .049) for all-cause mortality. For cancer and cardiovascular disease mortality, the HRs were 1.70 (95% CI = 1.01, 2.88, P = .049) and 1.47 (95% CI = .87, 2.47, P = .148) respectively.

Conclusions

Emotion suppression may convey risk for earlier death, including death from cancer. Further work is needed to better understand the biopsychosocial mechanisms for this risk, as well as the nature of associations between suppression and different forms of mortality.

Introduction

Emotion suppression, defined as a tendency to inhibit the expression of emotion [1], has long been suspected to influence health [2], with recent meta-analytic evidence linking suppression and chronic disease supportive of this long-held notion [3]. Emotion suppression involves intentionally avoiding distressing feelings by thinking of other things or holding things in, while emotion repression is defined by lack of conscious awareness of negative emotion [4], [5].

Suppression is believed to operate on health first at a behavioral level, by inducing unhealthy coping behaviors such as over-eating as substitutes for healthy emotional expression [6]. Second, at a physiological level, higher levels of autonomic reactivity to stress – measured both electrodermally and through blood pressure changes – have been reported among suppressors [7]. Direct correlations between suppressive defensive styles and both catecholamines and glucocorticoids have also been reported [8], [9] and are reviewed in [10], [11]. In turn, neuroendocrine dysregulation, whether induced by stress processes or habitual health-damaging behaviors, has been implicated in the progression of a number of chronic diseases, and ultimately earlier death [12].

Epidemiologic evidence for links between suppression and mortality appeared initially in a Yugoslavian cohort study conducted in the 1970 by Grossarth-Maticeck [13]. Specifically, a suppression-prone personality style called “anti-emotionality” predicted 10-year all-cause and Cardiovascular Disease (CVD) mortality [13]. In other work, Grossarth-Maticeck noted associations between this personality style and cancer death [13], [14]. Grossarth-Maticeck's studies were subsequently the center of controversy around data collection and analysis [15], [16], [17], [18], [19], [20], [21], [22], [23], [24], although he and his collaborator Hans Eysenck vigorously defended the results [25]. “Type C” personality style—also defined by a tendency to suppress emotion—was linked to poor health outcomes in the 1980s [26], [27], [28]. In the 1990s “Type D” personality, which involves affective distress in conjunction with social inhibition (presumably limiting emotional disclosure to others), was linked to both CVD death and numerous other health problems [29]. Other studies on the suppression of anger in particular have noted increased all-cause mortality over 17 years in a US community sample [30], 6 years in Dutch CVD patients [31], and 8.5 years in a German sample [32]. Yet there may also be a strong cultural contingency of suppression effects: in a Japanese community sample, lower levels of emotion suppression were linked to worse health [33], and in Japanese cancer patients, moderate, rather than high or low suppression levels were associated with survival [34]. The construct of repression conveyed a survival advantage in male veterans of the American army followed for 16 years [35], hinting at differential acculturation and gender variability in mortality risk of constructs in the suppression/repression family [6].

In short, while studies on the mortality risk of emotion suppression have been suggestive, they have been far from definitive, underscoring the need for new data in broadly diverse population samples. Our primary aim was to examine whether emotion suppression was associated with death from any cause over a 12-year follow-up in a nationally-representative US sample. We also assessed links between emotion suppression and the two leading specific causes of death in the US, CVD and cancer. Finally, we explored whether suppression of anger in particular [30], or other indicators of more specific types of suppression, were linked to mortality.

Section snippets

Sample and design

The General Social Survey (GSS) is an annual study of opinions and attitudes among the US public that is conducted by the National Opinion Research Center (NORC) at the University of Chicago. The survey uses a multi-stage probability sampling of non-institutionalized adults aged 18 and over, with response rates from 70% to 82% in any given year [36]. Interviews are conducted in person and involve a core set of questions asked every year (note that different people are included each year, so the

Results

Table 1 shows the sample demographics, which were similar to the 1996 US population estimates from the decennial census [49]. Mortality rates were in line with trends for that period reported by the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention[50]. The CVD death category was dominated by myocardial infarction, coronary atherosclerosis, and other ill-defined heart disease (together, 58% of CVD deaths). Leukemia, lung, pancreatic, and colon cancer accounted for 47% of the cancer deaths. The GSS'

Conclusions

Our analysis of a US nationally representative sample, followed for 12 years for mortality by cause of death, revealed significant associations between higher levels of emotion suppression and all-cause as well as cancer-related mortality. These findings have several implications. Theoretically, suppression is presumed to promote unhealthy behaviors as a substitute for appropriate emotional expression, and possibly engender neuroendocrine dysregulation [2], [51]. However, whether any such

Competing interest

None to declare.

References (75)

  • D Spiegel et al.

    Effect of psychosocial treatment on survival of patients with metastatic breast cancer

    Lancet

    (1989)
  • HM van der Ploeg et al.

    Rationality and antiemotionality as a risk factor for cancer: concept differentiation

    J Psychosom Res

    (1989)
  • KL Weihs et al.

    Negative affectivity, restriction of emotions, and site of metastases predict mortality in recurrent breast cancer

    J Psychosom Res

    (2000)
  • M Watson et al.

    Influence of psychological response on survival in breast cancer: a population-based cohort study

    Lancet

    (1999)
  • M Watson et al.

    Influence of psychological response on breast cancer survival: 10-year follow-up of a population-based cohort

    Eur J Cancer

    (2005)
  • J Kauhanen et al.

    Alexithymia and risk of death in middle-aged men

    J Psychosom Res

    (1996)
  • P Muennig et al.

    Socioeconomic status as an independent predictor of physiological biomarkers of cardiovascular disease: evidence from NHANES

    Prev Med

    (2007)
  • R GrossarthMaticek et al.

    Personality type, smoking habit and their interaction as predictors of cancer and coronary heart disease

    Personal Individ Differ

    (1988)
  • G Vaillant

    Ego mechanisms of defense: a guide for clinicians and researchers

    (1992)
  • PR Duberstein et al.

    Psychodynamic perspectives on sickness and health

    (2000)
  • M Mund et al.

    The costs of repression: a meta-analysis on the relation between repressive coping and somatic diseases

    Health Psychol

    (2011)
  • J Giese-Davis et al.

    Suppression, repressive-defensiveness, restraint, and distress in metastatic breast cancer: separable or inseparable constructs?

    J Pers

    (2001)
  • NS Consedine et al.

    Moderators of the emotion-inhibition–health relationship: a review and research agenda

    Rev Gen Psychol

    (2002)
  • P Cramer

    Stress, autonomic nervous system reactivity, and defense mechanisms

  • JSS Giese-Davis et al.

    Repression and high anxiety are associated with aberrant diurnal cortisol rhythms in women with metastatic breast cancer

    Health Psychol

    (2004)
  • U Hentschel et al.

    Defense mechanisms and their psychophysiological correlates

    Defense mechanisms: theoretical, research, and clinical perspectives

    (2004)
  • R Grossarth-Maticek

    Psychosocial predictors of cancer and internal diseases. An overview

    Psychother Psychosom

    (1980)
  • H Vetter

    Some observations on Grossarth-Maticek's data base

    Psychol Inq

    (1991)
  • HM Van der Ploeg

    What a wonderful world it would be: a reanalysis of some of the work of Grossarth-Maticek

    Psychol Inq

    (1991)
  • L Temoshok

    Assessing the assessment of psychosocial factors

    Psychol Inq

    (1991)
  • D Spiegel

    Second thoughts on personality, stress, and disease

    Psychol Inq

    (1991)
  • G Schuler et al.

    Questions about Grossarth-Maticek's procedures and results

    Psychol Inq

    (1991)
  • P Lee

    Personality and disease: a call for replication

    Psychol Inq

    (1991)
  • BH Fox

    Quandaries created by unlikely numbers in some of Grossarth-Maticek's studies

    Psychol Inq

    (1991)
  • LR Derogatis

    Personality, stress, disease, and bias in epidemiologic research

    Psychol Inq

    (1991)
  • M Amelang

    Tales from Crvenka and Heidelberg: what about the empirical basis?

    Psychol Inq

    (1991)
  • Cited by (58)

    • Positive and negative emotional expression measured from a single written essay about trauma predicts survival 17 years later in people living with HIV

      2020, Journal of Psychosomatic Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      Suppressed anger predicted both all cause and cardiovascular mortality in 696 people followed for 17 years [12]. A more recent study followed a nationally representative US adult sample of 729 individuals over 12 years and found that high emotional suppression predicted higher all-cause mortality and higher cancer mortality [13]. One study links inhibition of emotion with poorer health outcomes in PLWH.

    • Doing meta-work to navigate conventionally masculinist careers and work norms in professional workplaces

      2023, Routledge Handbook on Men, Masculinities and Organizations: Theories, Practices and Futures of Organizing
    View all citing articles on Scopus

    Support: US National Institutes of Health grants K08031328 (BC), 1RC2MD004768 (PM).

    View full text